Published
2024-11-27
Section
Research Articles
License
The journal adopts the Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International (CC BY-NC 4.0), which means that anyone can reuse and redistribute the materials for non-commercial purposes as long as you follow the license terms and the original source is properly cited.
Author(s) shall retain the copyright of their work and grant the Journal/Publisher rights for the first publication with the work concurrently licensed since 2023 Vol.8 No.2.
Under this license, author(s) will allow third parties to download, reuse, reprint, modify, distribute and/or copy the content under the condition that the authors are given credit. No permission is required from the authors or the publisher.
This broad license intends to facilitate free access, as well as the unrestricted use of original works of all types. This ensures that the published work is freely and openly available in perpetuity.
By providing open access, the following benefits are brought about:
- Higher Visibility, Availability and Citations-free and unlimited accessibility of the publication over the internet without any restrictions increases citation of the article.
- Ease of search-publications are easily searchable in search engines and indexing databases.
- Rapid Publication – accepted papers are immediately published online.
- Available for free download immediately after publication at https://esp.as-pub.com/index.php/ESP
Copyright Statement
1.The authors certify that the submitted manuscripts are original works, do not infringe the rights of others, are free from academic misconduct and confidentiality issues, and that there are no disputes over the authorship scheme of the collaborative articles. In case of infringement, academic misconduct and confidentiality issues, as well as disputes over the authorship scheme, all responsibilities will be borne by the authors.
2. The author agrees to grant the Editorial Office of Environment and Social Psychology a licence to use the reproduction right, distribution right, information network dissemination right, performance right, translation right, and compilation right of the submitted manuscript, including the work as a whole, as well as the diagrams, tables, abstracts, and any other parts that can be extracted from the work and used in accordance with the characteristics of the journal. The Editorial Board of Environment and Social Psychology has the right to use and sub-licence the above mentioned works for wide dissemination in print, electronic and online versions, and, in accordance with the characteristics of the periodical, for the period of legal protection of the property right of the copyright in the work, and for the territorial scope of the work throughout the world.
3. The authors are entitled to the copyright of their works under the relevant laws of Singapore, provided that they do not exercise their rights in a manner prejudicial to the interests of the Journal.
About Licence
Environment and Social Psychology is an open access journal and all published work is available under the Creative Commons Licence, Authors shall retain copyright of their work and grant the journal/publisher the right of first publication, and their work shall be licensed under the Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International (CC BY-NC 4.0).
Under this licence, the author grants permission to third parties to download, reuse, reprint, modify, distribute and/or copy the content with attribution to the author. No permission from the author or publisher is required.
This broad licence is intended to facilitate free access to and unrestricted use of original works of all kinds. This ensures that published works remain free and accessible in perpetuity. Submitted manuscripts, once accepted, are immediately available to the public and permanently accessible free of charge on the journal’s official website (https://esp.as-pub.com/index.php/ESP). Allowing users to read, download, copy, print, search for or link to the full text of the article, or use it for other legal purposes. However, the use of the work must retain the author's signature, be limited to non-commercial purposes, and not be interpretative.
Click to download <Agreement on the Licence for the Use of Copyright on Environmental and Social Psychology>.
How to Cite
Metric and temporal relationships in collaborative map drawings
Bernard Guelton
Université Paris 1 Panthéon Sorbonne, 75005 Paris, France
DOI: https://doi.org/10.59429/esp.v9i10.2928
Keywords: collaborative map, landmarks, graphs, spatial memorization, temporal memorization, group dynamics, collaborative memory inhibition, transactive memory
Abstract
While a number of studies have distinguished spatial and temporal memorization at individual level, none seem to have examined these two memorization modes in collaborative map drawing. After an initial review of the distinctions between spatial and temporal memorization at individual level, and a study of inhibition in collaborative memorization, we present several analyses carried out on 24 collaborative drawings from an urban spatial exploration. We compare metric and temporal measurements on these drawings to identify possible relationships between metric and temporal approaches, depending on whether group members proceeded individually or collectively in the urban exploration prior to the collaborative drawing phase.
- Based on 6 landmarks common to all drawings, an initial approach to metric and temporal measurements is carried out by comparing the distances obtained from the graphs created on the drawings, with the temporal measurements deduced from the video recordings. While the metric measures correlate well with the physical space (Google), they neither enable us to observe significant relationships between metric and temporal distances, nor to discriminate sufficiently between the groups.
- A second approach has therefore been taken, this time comparing the distances obtained from the starting point of urban exploration with landmarks’ order of appearance in the videos. In this case, the correlations obtained between metric distances and landmarks’ order of appearance prove to be significant for the group that interacted collectively in the urban space, but not for the groups that explored individually. Nevertheless, the group that repeated the collective exploration a month later showed relative independence from metric distances, in favor of a more global representation of the environment.
Author Biography
Bernard Guelton, Université Paris 1 Panthéon Sorbonne, 75005 Paris, France
Professor Université Paris 1 Panthéon SorbonneReferences
[1]. Kampis, D. & Southgate, V. (2020). Altercentric Cognition: How Others Influence Our Cognitive Processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24(11), 945-959. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2020.09.003.
[2]. Schafer, M., and Schiller, D. (2018). Navigating social space. Neuron 100, 476–489. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.10.006.
[3]. Tavares, R. M., Mendelsohn, A., Grossman, Y., Williams, C. H., Shapiro, M., Trope, Y. et Schiller, D. (2015). A Map for Social Navigation in the Human Brain. Neuron. Volume 87, Issue 1, 1 July 2015, Pages 231-243.
[4]. Dalton, R. C., Holscher, C., & Montello, D. R. (2019). Wayfinding as a social activity. Frontiers in Psychology, JO, 142.
[5]. Bae, C. J., & Montello, D. R. (2019). Dyadic route planning and navigation in collaborative wayfinding. In S. Timpf, C. Schlieder, M. Kattenbeck, B. Ludwig, & K. Stewart (Eds.), Proceedings of the 14th international conference on spatial information theory (pp. 1- 20). LIPICS.
[6]. He, G., Ishikawa, T. et Takemiya, M. (2015). Collaborative navigation in an unfamiliar environment with people having different spatial aptitudes, Spatial Cognition & Computation, 15(4), 285-307. https://doi.org/10.1080/13875868.2015.1072537.
[7]. Buck L., E., et al. (2020). Determining Peripersonal Space Boundaries and Their Plasticity in Relation to Object and Agent Characteristics in an Immersive Virtual Environment, 2020 IEEE Conference on Virtual Reality and 3D User Interfaces (VR).
[8]. Reilly, D., Mackay, B., Watters, C. (2009). Planners, navigators, and pragmatists: collaborative wayfinding using a single mobile phone. Pers Ubiquit Comput 13, 321–329 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00779-008-0207-2.
[9]. Simon, M., A., (2004). Many wrongs: the advantage of group navigation. Trends in ecology and evolution. Vol. 19. N° 9.
[10]. Dorfman, A., Weiss, O., Hagbi, Z., Levi, A. et Eilam, D. (2021). Social spatial cognition. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 121, 277- 290.
[11]. Dyer, J., R., G., Ioannou, C., C., Morrell, L., J., Croft, D., P., Krause, Couzin, I., D., Waters, D., A., Krause, J. (2008). Consensus decision making in human crowds. Animal Behaviour. Volume 75, Issue 2, February 2008, Pages 461-470.
[12]. Faria, J., Edward A. Codling, E. A., Dyer, J. R.G., Fritz Trillmich, F., Krause, J. (2009) Navigation in human crowds; testing the many-wrongs principle, Animal Behaviour 78 (2009) 587-591.
[13]. Helbing, D., Molnár, P., Farkas, I. J., & Bolay, K. (2001). Self-Organizing Pedestrian Movement. Environment and Planning B: Planning and Design, 28(3), 361-383. https://doi.org/10.1068/b2697.
[14]. Curtin K., M. & Montello D. R. (2024). Collective spatial cognition, a research agenda, Routledge.
[15]. Rajaram, S. (2011). Collaboration both hurts and helps memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(2) 76-81. https:// doi.org/10.1177/0963721411403251.
[16]. Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
[17]. Vygotksy, L. S. (1978). Mind in Society, Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.
[18]. Weldon, M. S. (2001). Remembering as a social process. In D. L. Medin (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory, Vol. 40, pp. 67–120). Academic Press.
[19]. Weldon, M. S., & Bellinger, K. D. (1997). Collective memory: Collaborative and individual processes in remembering. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23.
[20]. Basden, B. H., Basden, D. R., Bryner, S., & Thomas, R. L. III. (1997). A comparison of group and individual remembering: Does collaboration disrupt retrieval strategies? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23(5), 1176,1189. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.23.5.1176.
[21]. Bays PM, Wu EY, Husain M. Storage and binding of object features in visual working memory. Neuropsychologia. 2011 May;49(6):1622-31. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.023. Epub 2010 Dec 21. PMID: 21172364; PMCID: PMC3119435.
[22]. Congleton, A. E. & Rajaram, S. (2014). Collaboration Changes Both the Content and the Structure of Memory: Building the Architecture of Shared Representations. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General © 2014 American Psychological Association, 2014, Vol. 143, No. 4, 1570-1584.
[23]. Maupin, C. K., Maclaren, N. G., Goodwin, G., Carter, D. R., (2024). Improving wayfinding through transactive memory systems, in Collective spatial cognition, a research agenda, Edited by Kevin M. Curtin & Daniel R. Montello; Routledge, Taylor & Francis group.
[24]. DeChurch, L. A., & Mesmer-Magnus, J. R. (2010). The cognitive underpinnings of effective teamwork: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(1), 32–53. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017328.
[25]. Austin, J. (2003). Transactive Memory in Organizational Groups: The Effects of Content, Consensus, Specialization, and Accuracy on Group Performance. The Journal of applied psychology. 88. 866-78. 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.866.
[26]. Lewis, K. (2003). Measuring transactive memory systems in the field: Scale development and validation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(4), 587.
[27]. Wegner, D. M. (1986). Transactive memory: A contemporary analysis of the group mind. In B. Mullen & G. R. Goethals (Eds.), Theories of group behavior (pp. 185–205). New York: Springer-Verlag.
[28]. Liang, D.W., Moreland, R.L., & Argote, L. (1995). Group Versus Individual Training and Group Performance: The Mediating Role of Transactive Memory. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 384 - 393.
[29]. Moreland, R. L. (1999). Transactive memory: Learning who knows what in work groups and organizations. In L. L. Thompson , J. M. Levine , & D. M. Messick (Eds.), Shared cognition inorganizations: The management of knowledge (pp. 3–31). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
[30]. Moreland, R. L. , Argote, L. , & Krishnan, R. (1996). Socially shared cognition at work: Transactive memory and group performance. In J. L. Nye & A. M. Brower (Eds.), What’s so social about social cognition? Social cognition research in small groups (pp. 57–84). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
[31]. Curiel, J. M., & Radvansky, G. A. (2002). Mental maps in memory retrieval and comprehension. Memory, 10(2), 113–126. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210143000245.
[32]. Curiel, J. M., & Radvansky, G. A. (1998). Mental organization of maps. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24(1), 202–214. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.24.1.202.
[33]. Penaud, S., Jebara, N., Zaoui, M., Orriol, E., Berthoz, A., Piolino, P. (2022). Episodic memory and self-reference in a naturalistic context: new insights based on a virtual walk in the Latin Quarter of Paris, Journal of Environmental Psychology 81 (2022) 101801.
[34]. Ekstrom, A., D, & Bookheimer, S., Y. (2007). Spatial and temporal episodic memory retrieval recruit dissociable functional networks in the human brain. Learning memory. Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press.
[35]. Mc Namara, T., P., Halpin, J., A., Hardy, J., K. (1992). Spatial and temporal contributions to the structure of spatial memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(3), 555–564. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.18.3.555.
[36]. Clark, D., P., Bruno, D. (2021). Time is of the essence: Exploring temporal and spatial organization in episodic memory, Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). 2021 Aug; 74(8): 1406-1417. Published online 2021 Feb 25. doi: 10.1177/1747021821993823.
[37]. Rajaram, S. (2017). Collaborative Inhibition in Group Recall: Cognitive Principles and Implications. Meade, M., L., Ed., Collaborative Remembering: Theories, Research, and Applications. Oxford University Press.
[38]. Brauner E., Becker A. (2006). Beyond knowledge sharing: the management of transactive knowledge systems Knowledge and Process Management. 13: 62-71. DOI: 10.1002/Kpm.240.